Dancing in the City

Beekeeping is an increasingly popular pastime. Since ~84% of the UK population live in urban areas (up from ~70% in 1950’s) it is not unsurprising that the number of hives kept in cities is increasing.

Of course, not everyone who lives in a town or city keeps their bees in the back garden. When I lived in the Midlands I lived on a small estate that was indisputably ‘urban’, although there was farmland within sight 1. My bees were on the nearby farmland and I just kept a few mating nucs and a bait hive in the back garden 2

Hive in a field margin

I kept my bees in the farmland because 3 I reasoned that there were both larger amounts and a greater range of forage available for them there.

But I was probably wrong.

It wouldn’t be the first time, and it certainly won’t be the last.

Too many bees?

Before discussing urban bees and forage in more detail I’ll digress a minute to mention the suggestions that the inexorable rise and rise of urban beekeeping is threatening our native pollinators.

Actually … more than suggestions.

There are a number of scientific reports and reviews that indicate that urban beekeeping harms – by outcompeting – native pollinators like solitary bees. A recent report by the Royal Botanic Gardens at Kew states:

‘Campaigns encouraging people to save bees have resulted in an unsustainable proliferation in urban beekeeping. This approach only saves one species of bee, the honeybee, with no regard for how honeybees interact with other, native species.’

‘In some places, such as London, so many people have established urban hives that the honeybee populations are threatening other bee species.’

Our bees (Apis mellifera) are generalists. They are not particularly well adapted to any flower or nectar/pollen source.

They are equal opportunists.

Individually, there are many solitary bee species or non-hymenopteran pollinators, that are ‘better’ adapted. They pollinate more efficiently, or collect more nectar, faster.

But honey bees arrive in the environment mob handed.

Thousands of them.

Actually, tens or hundreds of thousands of them if there are several hives in an apiary. They are a formidable force and can easily outcompete other pollinators that are either solitary or only live in small colonies.

Save the bees, save humanity

When you see the phrase “Save the bees” what it usually means is save the honey bees.

Save the bees ...

Save the bees …

What it should be encouraging is “Save anything but the honey bees, because they don’t need saving … actually, there are possibly too many of them altogether”.

But that’s a lot less catchy … and it won’t let the supermarket, or food manufacturer or whatever, illustrate their campaign with cute photos of pollen-laden honey bees returning to quaint white-painted WBC hives in some sort of idyllic rural scene 4.

I suppose a photo of a honey bee would be better than drawing of a wasp though … which is what the NRDC used for a (mis)information campaign on CCD (colony collapse disorder) a few years ago.

D’oh!

Anyway … saving the bees is usually “greenwash” or bee-washing as it was termed by MacIvor and Packer in a 2015 paper on bee hotels 5

I’ll return to this topic, and urban beekeeping, later in the winter 6.

The Town mouse bees and the Country mouse bees

Where was I?

Oh yes, my – as will shortly be clear – incorrect assumption that country bees have access to more diverse and richer sources of forage than their poor relatives living in the town.

There are many sorts of countryside and many sorts of urban environments.

A hive in an intensively farmed arable landscape could be located in hundreds of acres of wheat fields, where all of the hedgerows were grubbed up years ago and replaced – if they weren’t just removed altogether – with barbed wire 7

How different is that environment to the ‘concrete jungle’ of a modern city? 

Tokyo

Surely that must be a terrible environment for bees?

In contrast, the suburban sprawl that surrounds most cities is possibly a good place for bees to live. Lots of neat little gardens, each 8 with a profusion of flowering plants, each chosen to provide vibrant colour for a much longer period than native plants.

And I’m sure we can all think of forage-rich rural environments. Here the bees gorge on early crocus, then gorse, willow, oil seed rape, clover, lime, blackberry, fireweed and himalayan balsam, before finishing the season will full crops and corbiculae from the ivy.

Now, in a recent publication 9 scientists have compared the forage available to town and country bees, and the results are quite surprising.

Let the bees tell you

If you live in a country with enlightened right to roam laws (like Scotland) you could wander around the countryside recording all the forage available to bees.

But the laws in England are much less enlightened. However, your right to roam in either country does not extend to the land around a private house or building. 

So, although you might be able to determine the forage available in the Scottish countryside, you can’t be certain you would have good enough access to do the same in England. And in a city you could only map what forage was available by peering over fences from public roads. 

So Elli Leadbetter and colleagues let the bees do the work.

They established 20 observation hives. Ten were in the the centre of London and 10 in agricultural land to the North East and South West of London. The hives were 5 km from each other to avoid overlapping areas of forage. They used observation hives so that they could watch and record the waggle dances of foragers in the hives.

I’ve discussed the waggle dance before. It is used to communicate three important pieces of information about a forage source:

  • direction
  • distance
  • quality

The first two bits of information are encoded in the angle of the waggle run to the perpendicular and the duration of the waggle run. The quality is conveyed by the number of circuits a dancing working performs. It’s a case of ‘the more, the better’ … energetically higher quality resources 10 result in more circuits.

Having recorded thousands of these waggle dances, they used the direction and distance information to ‘map’ where the bees were foraging.

GIS data, land use and foraging bees

For many locations there exists a wealth of land use data (GIS data; Geographic Information Systems). Much of this is at high resolution. For each of the 20 observation hives they produced a map of land use within 2.5 km of the hive at a resolution of 25 m. 

Land use was defined in broad categories such as 11 buildings, woodland, arable, pasture, fruit, OSR (all in agricultural areas) and dense or sparse residential, parks, amenity grassland or railways (all in urban landscapes). 

They then used some clever mathematics to decode the waggle dances 12 to work out where the bees had been, converting the distance and direction components to geographic locations.

Urban (top) and rural foraging heat maps, overlaid on GIS land use maps (5 km diameter)

These locations were overlaid on the land use maps to produce ‘heat maps’ showing where the bees were foraging. The image above shows these heat maps. In the spring the urban bees (top left) were foraging intensively to the east and west of the hive and the rural bees (bottom left) were mainly foraging in two large areas to the south east and south west of the hive.

Foraging distance and nectar quality

Even a cursory look at the image above shows that the urban bees tended to forage closer to the hive than the rural bees. But remember, that’s just three snapshots during the season.

Waggle run duration – rural bees fly further

However, more detailed analysis confirmed that this was the case. Throughout the season, the bees in the agricultural landscape foraged further from the hive. I’m showing the log-transformed median waggle run duration (above) as this allows slightly easier comparison across the season. The further the wiggly ‘best fit’ line is above the horizontal axis, the longer the duration of the waggle dance run, and so the further the bees are flying to find the forage.

Interestingly, the median foraging distances were relatively short when compared with the maximum foraging distances from the decoded waggle dances. This applied wherever the bees lived. For urban bees the median was 492 m (max 9375 m) and for agricultural bees it was 743 m (max 8158 m 13 ).

Perhaps the agricultural bees were flying further because there was better quality nectar available at more distant sites?

Nectar concentration (% w/w) sampled from returning workers

They controlled for this by recovering returning foragers and robbing them of their nectar load before analysing the sugar content. There was no significant difference 14 between the nectar from agricultural or urban landscapes. The sugar content of the nectar was recorded as reducing through the season.

Foraging preferences

So where did the town bees and the country bees prefer to forage?

City rooftop bees

City rooftop bees …

In urban areas the bees exhibited a strong preference for residential gardens (the ‘sparse residential land’) … these are presumably the flower-rich urban gardens that the homeowners also tend to prefer.

In contrast, bees in an agricultural landscape showed an entirely unsurprising reliance upon mass-flowering crops like oil seed rape (in spring only). They also showed weaker preferences for arable land and fruit crops throughout the season.

Mid-April in the apiary ...

Mid-April in a Warwickshire apiary …

I’ve skipped over a host of additional observations from the study, and almost all of the controls. Two things that are worth mentioning though.

Firstly, hive strength had no influence on waggle dance duration (and hence foraging distance). It therefore wasn’t the case that stronger hives had a larger workforce that could survey and exploit more distant forage. 

Secondly, cities are warmer due to the urban heat island effect. However, temperature also did not affect waggle dance duration when it was factored in. So, the city bees aren’t foraging at shorter distances because the dance is truncated at higher temperatures.

Conclusions

So, although cities are predominantly filled with buildings and roads, city bees travel less far to forage when compared to bees in agricultural landscapes.

This strongly suggests that the urban landscape consistently provided more available forage 15

Conversely, the bees in agricultural landscapes had to fly further, not because there were better quality nectar sources available at long-range, but because there wasn’t enough nectar nearby.

There were a number of additional interesting points in the paper, some of which were known already from other studies. For example, the high sugar content in spring nectar was already known (and confirmed here). Similarly, foraging distances in midsummer were longer than those in spring or autumn. This could be predicted due to the reduced rainfall in summer, and consequently the reduced overall level of nectar available.

I need to think more about how this study contributes – if at all – to the ‘too many bees in cities’ argument. If anything, forage-rich towns should be able to support a greater number of bees 16 without the honey bees impacting on other species. In contrast, honey bees in agricultural landscapes might dominate the available nectar sources because they can forage at long distances and then communicate to their nestmates the location.

Perhaps it just shows that that heavily farmed land is actually very poor in terms of nectar availability? It’s either boom or bust … once the OSR has finished, or the clover has been ploughed in, or the fruit trees stop flowering then there’s nothing left 🙁

It would be interesting to conduct a similar study in a forage-rich, non-agricultural, rural landscape.

Access all areas

Finally, I think a particularly neat thing about this study is the use of bees to ‘map’ the forage availability using what the authors term “a large-scale search effort that has no access limitations”. The scientists interpreted the waggle dance to get information that would otherwise have been difficult or impossible to determine.

However, using the bee’s own perception of the distances flown might actually lead to inaccuracies in the calculations. As discussed previously, bees measure distance by optic flow. Optic flow increases in complex landscapes … and cities are likely (at least to us, and certainly when compared with arable farmland, to bees) to be complex landscapes. Increased optic flow leads to a perception of increased distance, and hence to longer waggle runs.

This means that bees in complex landscapes might overestimate the distance they have actually flown to forage. Conversely, those in uniform landscapes might underestimate.

Which means that the results of this study may be a conservative estimate of the differences in foraging distance.

And therefore a conservative estimate of the differences in forage availability in urban and agricultural landscapes.


Notes

Dancing in the City was a pretty cheesy song that reached #3 in the charts for the rock-pop duo Marshall Hain in 1978. Having remembered the track when thinking up a title for this post, I made the mistake of listening to it on Spotify.

I now can’t get the damned thing out of my head. 

But it gets worse. I rummaged through Wikipedia and discovered that Kit Hain, the vocalist, subsequently had a very successful songwriting career with people like Roger Daltrey, Chaka Khan and Fleetwood Mac. Impressive. 

In contrast, Julian Marshall, the keyboard player became a member of the Flying Lizards who had a minor hit with a cover version of Barrett Strong’s 17 Money (That’s What I Want)

… and now I can’t get that out of my head 🙁

Footnotes

  1. From an upstairs window, if you stood on tiptoe … with binoculars.
  2. Which was, without fail, successful every year.
  3. Based on the unedifying small patch of turf in my garden.
  4. Don’t they know we all use garishly coloured polystyrene hives these days?
  5. MacIvor JS, Packer L (2015) ‘Bee Hotels’ as Tools for Native Pollinator Conservation: A Premature Verdict?. PLOS ONE 10(3): e0122126. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122126
  6. If I remember …
  7. Never much good for nesting birds or foraging bees.
  8. Or at least most, with the exception of mine.
  9. Samuelson, A. E.Schürch, R., & Leadbeater, E. (2021). Dancing bees evaluate central urban forage resources as superior to agricultural landJournal of Applied Ecology001– 10https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2664.14011
  10. Taking account of the energy invested in travel and the energy gained in food intake.
  11. These are incomplete lists just to give you a flavour of the types of land use they defined.
  12. 2800 of them in total, recorded fortnightly between April and September.
  13. These maxima are extrapolated, not actual distances … it seems unlikely a forager would fly a total of almost 19 km.
  14. The ‘best fit’ lines and their errors are almost superimposed in the graph above.
  15. I told you I was wrong (yet again) in the opening paragraphs.
  16. Whether honey bees, bumble bees or solitary bees.
  17. Of Tamla Motown fame.

18 thoughts on “Dancing in the City

  1. Janet Wilson

    A most interesting reflection on the issues around urban beekeeping. I live in Canada, and a few years ago a large piece of public land was going to be developed for community use. Bees made it into the final plan, but not before town council had to answer charges that the (non native to the Americas) honey bees were usurping forage and hence native bee populations were under threat. My advice to the council was to consider that for 500+ years, honey bees and native pollinators thrived together on this continent (N. America). There were loads of both. Now they are both struggling and in Canada we think there are no more truly feral honey bee colonies. What has changed? Well, loss of forage and habitat via development, agrisprays/landscaping sprays and (in the case of honey bees) Varroa mites. You could vanish the honey bees overnight and native pollinators would still struggle with starvation, habitat destruction and toxic sprays. What needs to be done is fuel a rising tide that lifts ALL boats: greenspace, forage trails, organic agriculture and landscaping. If we supplied this improvements to the landscape, all pollinators would rebound and thrive.

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hello Janet

      I agree … the combination of intensive agriculture, monoculture, pesticide sprays and diseases has had a devastating impact on bees, and insects more generally.

      I now live in a truly wild environment. I’d be surprised if any pesticides are used – perhaps other than on the odd rose bush – and there are certainly no arable crops for 20-30 miles. The ‘garden’ (it’s just rough land around us) is full of bumble bees and butterflies through the spring and summer. The contrast between this area and the agricultural east coast is really marked. We don’t even have Varroa here. However, the climate here is probably borderline for honey bees and my searches for wild/feral colonies has yet to be successful (though I know of two colonies cut out of a roof space that had apparently been there for ‘years’). My colony numbers here will never be excessive … I don’t want to have to feed lots of colonies during the season if there’s no nectar, and I don’t want to crowd out the natives. We have a lot of rhododendron here. The honey bees don’t touch it, but it’s alive with bumble bees when in flower.

      There are some glimmers of hope … rewilding projects are on the increase, with one very large one in the highlands of Scotland. Whether these will prove to be ‘too little, too late’ will become clear over the next few decades.

      How depressing 🙁

      David

      Reply
      1. Janet Wilson

        I think it is easier than we fear…if the local farms not only went organic (something that I think is inevitable as more and more deleterious human health effects are found to be attributable to agrisprays), but also planted hedgerows again, that alone would create a wealth of safe habitat and forage. The fact that urban landscapes provide more and better forage than “rural” comes as a very pleasant surprise. If you plant it, they will rebound. Now Varroa…that is another matter that I expect will be solved via gene manipulation technology. We have to eradicate Varroa from all honey bee stocks, world-wide.

        Reply
        1. David Post author

          I agreed about the hedgerows, but that increases the harvesting costs and we haven’t got anyone to do the work here now anyway (thanks Brexit!) … and we are wedded to cheap food. But something certainly has to change.

          The paper specifically contrasted urban with agricultural. I know I used rural once or twice, but I tried to apply the appropriate emphasis when needed. I suspect lots of their rural landscapes looked like this:

          Nothing here for the bees

          … which, when I used that photo before, I’d labelled ‘Fife, not Kansas’.

          I’m sure Varroa can be solved via gene manipulation, but am not sure it will be. The current interest in ‘treatment free’ (whatever the cost) is very much pushing in the other direction. Years ago we looked at the patent landscape for GM in honey bees. The consultants we employed suggested that there were lots of opportunities in the US (largely because of the $4bn almond business) but that Europe was a complete non-starter. I think the acceptable target would be to genetically engineer Varroa rather than honey bees … similar in overall principle to flooding the area with sterile male mosquitoes. Surely no-one could take exception to a dominant negative Varroa being released 😉

          Cheers
          David

          Reply
  2. Dave Stokes

    “recovering returning foragers and robbing them of their nectar load” sounds painful.
    Thanks for an interesting article, as usual.
    Dave.

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hi Dave

      All achieved non-destructively 🙂 Here’s the relevant bit from the Materials & Methods; “… we collected nectar sucrose concentration data by blocking the entrance to the hive and collecting 10 returning foragers that were not carrying pollen. Following anaesthesia in a cool bag containing ice blocks, we stimulated re-gurgitation by massaging the bees’ abdomens with forceps. Using a microcapillary tube, crop contents were transferred to a 0–80° Brix refractometer (Kern) to measure sucrose concentration.”

      No bees were harmed in the making of this interesting publication.

      Cheers
      David

      Reply
  3. Joss Bartlett

    Thanks – very interesting: analysis of bee dances always sounds like one of those things that’s simple in principle but must take so long to do. But I could happily have gone through the rest of my life without ever hearing ‘Dancing in the City’ again, and now it’s going to play in my head all day. Thanks for that.
    Joss

    Reply
  4. Richard Searle

    Hi David

    I read your article with particular interest having started on Beekeeping in the urban setting of North Manchester in 2012 before moving to rustic Hebden Bridge a few years ago

    Urban beekeeping throws up some interesting results.

    In 2014 a University of Manchester student for her dissertation carried out pollen analysis on the honey from our hives in North Manchester and compared with this honey with that from the roof top hives of Manchester Art Gallery and Manchester Museum in the City Centre.

    The city centre honey had a dominance of tree pollen and not flowers.

    The North Manchester honey had what you would expect from hives based on an allotment, such a, brambles and brassica pollen but it also had cannabis pollen in it

    This results supposes that some of those bees had managed to find ways to breach the ‘security’ of a local cannabis farm.

    We can only speculate how the Bees detected this pollen source suffice to say that the North Manchester Honey did not have any psychotropic qualities.

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hello Richard

      Cannabis? There’s a surprise (er, or not!). However, not surprising that it wasn’t psychotropic as pollen contains “much lower levels of the intoxicating cannabinoid THC“.

      What’s also not a surprise is that the city centre samples were rich with tree pollen. In true city centres they are often the only flowering plant, though initiatives like the Bristol’s Pollinator Strategy are trying to change that – and showing the benefits.

      Hebden Bridge presumably has lots of managed moorland around it … good for heather honey, but less good for the flooding.

      I’m hoping to write a bit more about urban pollinators this winter if I get a chance to do my ‘homework’ reading 🙂

      Cheers
      David

      Reply
      1. Richard Searle

        Thanks for the David

        Certainly different in West Yorkshire – where this year Heather honey is cropping up everywhere making extracting a right pain in the proverbial.

        One similarity between the Calder Valley and Manchester is the Balsam which flourishes on old industrial landscapes

        We made this short film 7 years ago about the roof top hives in Manchester City centre as part of an idea of creating ‘pollinator friendly cities’ which if it could be done in places like Manchester & Salford it could be done anywhere.

        https://youtu.be/H5LISIxsww4

        It’s no oscar winner but did allow us access to areas the public normally don’t get to see

        Has some top notch piano playing !!

        Reply
        1. David Post author

          Hi Richard

          Yes, some interesting piano. Les Dawson perhaps? Not sure I’d want to carry full supers down those narrow stairs from the cathedral roof!

          Cheers
          David

          Reply
  5. Julian Robert Cox

    Thought provoking.
    I am a member of our local beekeeping association, in North Devon, where while rural, the majority of the agriculture is pasture for dairy or sheep, with some crops for dairy/beef – wheat, maize, and field beans, and the very occasional OSR. The association has an uneasy view of the expansion of beekeeping because of a perceived paucity of forage but we have no evidence. I also follow a beekeeping group on Facebook and am slightly horrified when someone puts up a post of how they want to give up the day job and keep 100, or 500 hives in a rural locality. The supporters say relax, the honeybees don’t compete with other pollinators. Thinking about my poor 4 hives struggling, but mores so, bumblebees etc, I would hate it if they moved their 100 hives in next door, Again, I have no evidence to give credence to my anxiety.
    Keep me posted!
    Thanks
    Julian

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hi Julian

      I suspect most people who make statements like that have absolutely no idea of the crippling amount of work involved in running several hundred colonies … which probably also means they have no clue about how to judge whether there’s sufficient forage in a particular area to support those colonies, or to support the colonies without being detrimental to other things in the environment. For most, I suspect it’s “just talk”.

      If they think running a hundred hives is anything like ‘sauntering down to the bottom of the garden on a balmy Saturday afternoon to check whether the queen has mated’, multiplied up a few times I suggest they check some of the beefarmers who post on Twitter (and they’re the ones with time to post – either because they employ others, or because they perhaps don’t run enough hives to actually make a healthy living). It’s not a big hobby, it’s industrial.

      I’ve been doing some reading on impact on other pollinator and need to read some more. Honey bees do compete with other pollinators, but as numerous generalists … it’s a combination of their abilities as foragers and the size of a colony.

      If your 4 hives are struggling then it suggests there’s not enough forage locally. Anyone dumping 100 colonies in the area would soon be out of business. Perhaps look for another site a few miles away and move a couple of hives there. The comparison is always interesting and informative, and it’s really useful having a second apiary for taking splits or swarms etc.

      Cheers
      David

      Reply
  6. James

    Yes, I’m still replying to old posts 😀

    You write “This means that bees in complex landscapes might overestimate the distance they have actually flown to forage. Conversely, those in uniform landscapes might underestimate.”, but I wonder if this may be anthropomorphising (is that even a word, and if so is it even the right one?) what’s going on.

    I wonder if the “distance” indicated by the dance is more likely to be described to other bees in some “unit” related to the rate of optic flow, meaning they’re not under- or over-estimating the distance at all. Is the error perhaps more likely to be ours in misinterpreting the dance to convey distance in units that are meaningful to humans, causing us to under- or over-estimate?

    I’ve read (one of?) your earlier pieces on the waggle dance (about some work by Jurgen Tautz, possibly?) suggesting the bees have no problem going the correct distance as long as they experience the same rate of optic flow, so presumably they all know what they’re up to even if we don’t.

    James

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hi James

      I had to go back and re-read the original post …

      ‘Distance’ in metres or kilometres is probably irrelevant here. What the bees do is dance to indicate the angle to set off at (with relation to the sun) and the ‘amount’ of optic flow to allow before stopping flying and starting foraging. Since the landscape is relatively invariant whatever the real distance the waggle dance translates to is irrelevant for the bees. They’ll be flying following the instructions they’re given. As long as the landscape doesn’t change they’ll get there.

      However, when the scientists interpret the dance the point I was trying to make is that waggle dances in complex environments (and it’s arguable whether a town is as far as a bee is concerned) are probably not directly comparable to those in ‘simple’ environments. A 2 second run in a complex environment does not indicate the same human distance as a 2 second run in a simple environment. Consequently, the distances the authors report might not be accurate.

      It’s an interesting question as what the bees actually measure … but that gets us into the minefield of neurobiology which I know even less about than I do beekeeping 😉

      Cheers
      David

      Reply
      1. James

        Yes, it’s that the humans are potentially misinterpreting the distances that is the issue I was getting at. The bees aren’t under- or over-estimating anything and get it right every time (as long as someone hasn’t, err, moved the goalposts). They’re just using units that we don’t have a reliable conversion factor for.

        James

        Reply
        1. David Post author

          Yes, the humans may misinterpret … if the bees did they’d go hungry! Our interpretations might not be the actual distances, but what matters is that there are apparent differences in the distances in rural and urban environments.

          Cheers
          David

          Reply

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *