The bees know best

Synopsis : Queens reared under the emergency response are numerous and preferentially started from eggs. The cells are then subjected to strong selection by workers after capping. What does this tell us about good quality queens and can we use this knowledge to improve our own queen rearing?

Introduction

In Eats, sleeps, bees I made a passing comment on the confidence I have in the ability of bees to choose ‘good’ larvae when rearing a new queen. I was justifying why I only leave a single queen cell in a colony that needs requeening. The precise words were:

“I also had total confidence that the bees had selected a suitable larva to raise as a queen in the first place. After all, the survival of the resulting colony depends on it.”

I thought this might be an interesting topic to look at in a little more detail. There is some interesting science on queen cell production.

And subsequent destruction.

Queen cells

Queen cells … have they chosen well?

In addition, there are related observations on what the bees choose as the starting material for queen cells. This should inform our own queen rearing activities. I’ll discuss these (briefly) after presenting the science.

Emergency, supersedure and swarm responses

But first I need introduce the three ‘responses’ under which a colony rears one or more new queens. These are the emergency, supersedure and swarming responses 1

The swarming response

Around this time of the season 2 many beekeepers will be familiar with queen cells produced under the impulse to swarm.

A strong, queenright colony runs out of space. Eggs are laid in specially created vertically oriented cells and are subsequently reared as new queens.

Once these swarm cells are sealed the colony swarms. The old queen and a significant proportion of the workers disappear over the fence. One or more new queens emerge and the colony may produce casts, each headed by a virgin queen. One new queen finally remains, gets mated and heads the original colony.

Swarming is honey bee reproduction … it is the only (natural) way one colony becomes two.

The supersedure response

Supersedure is the in situ replacement of the current queen. The colony produces a small number of supersedure cells – often just one, located in the middle of a central frame 3 – the new queen emerges, mates and starts laying. There may be two queens in the box for an extended period, but eventually the old queen disappears.

Supersedure is probably more common than most beekeepers think. It is the usual explanation for the presence of an unmarked queen at an early season inspection in a hive that had previously contained a marked queen.

The emergency response

If the incumbent queen is removed or killed the colony must rear another or they are doomed. They do this under the emergency response.

Some beekeepers – particularly beginners 4 – inadvertently crush the queen while returning brood frames. They are then surprised at the next inspection to find no eggs but a lot of queen cells.

What’s this? Swarming finished weeks ago!

This is the emergency response at work. The bees select several suitable eggs or larvae, reshape the comb to allow a vertically-oriented cell to be drawn and feed with copious amounts of Royal Jelly.

And voilà, a new queen 5 is produced.

Inducing these responses

The emergency response is triggered by the removal of the old queen – either by physically taking her out of the box, or killing her. Both are easy to achieve 🙁 6

There are ways to induce a supersedure response, but they sometimes involve damaging the queen 7 and are unreliable and – more importantly – ethically dubious. There are more ethically acceptable alternatives.

Lots of beekeepers inadvertently induce the swarming response by not providing the bees with sufficient space, not supering early enough or allowing the brood nest to be backfilled with nectar.

However, doing this in a controlled manner is not a certainty. In one of my apiaries 50% of the colonies have shown no inclination to swarm this season whereas the others all produced swarm cells. All were treated similarly and were – to all intents and purposes – of equivalent strength.

Sealed queen cells produced under the swarming response

For scientific purposes inducing a swarming response cannot be relied upon for studies of queen cell production and selection.

In contrast, the emergency response is 100% reliable. Therefore, in the majority of studies on brood choice, queen cell production and selection, it’s the emergency response that is exploited. That’s certainly the case with the two papers I’m going to briefly discuss this week. 8.

Pick a larva, any larva

Is that what the bees do?

Of course not.

Regular readers will remember from Timing is everything that only larva up to three days old are suitable for producing new queens i.e. six days after the egg is laid.

However, if the queen is laying 1000 eggs per day 9 that still means there are up to 3000 suitably aged larvae in the hive for the production of a new queen, should one be needed.

Eggs and young larvae

Eggs and young larvae

Actually there’s even more choice as the bees can start the queen rearing process – the production of a queen cell – from a cell occupied by an egg … something that has been known for decades, but is relatively rarely discussed.

So, what do they choose?

The first study I’m going to discuss addresses this point and the interesting (and critical) aspect of the quality of the resulting queens that are produced.

Hatch, S., Tarpy, D. & Fletcher, D. Worker regulation of emergency queen rearing in honey bee colonies and the resultant variation in queen quality. Insectes soc. 46, 372–377 (1999).

The study was very straightforward. They induced the emergency response by dequeening strong hives. They then monitored the production and position of queen cells over time, determining the age of the egg/larvae selected by extrapolating back from the day the queen cell was sealed.

Cells that were capped were caged with queen excluder and the resulting emerged queen was analysed to determine her quality. This essentially involved determining her size and weight (the bigger the better) and ovarial number, but they measured additional features as well.

Emergency cell production

In the 8 colonies used, almost all queen cell construction was started within 24 hours of queen removal. A few more cells were produced for up to 48 hours after dequeening, but none were started after that.

There will still be many hundreds of (apparently) suitably aged larvae in the colony at this point. However, these were not selected as all the queen cells that would be made had already been started.

Colonies produced different numbers of queen cells, from 6 to 56 (average 27).

However, the majority of these cells were torn down before emergence, and a few of those that were sealed never emerged. Of the 217 cells started, 115 (53%) were torn down, 11 (5%) did not emerge and the remaining 91 (43%) emerged.

Not only did the number of queen cells produced vary greatly between hives, so did the numbers of queens that emerged – from 3 to 20 (average 11).

The brood nest is roughly spherical or rugby ball-shaped and usually occupies the centre of the hive. About 46% of the cells started were on the central three frames, and these had a much greater chance of producing queens. This was because queen cells started on the central frames of the brood nest were less likely to be torn down (41%) than those on the periphery (71%).

Pick an egg or a larva (in which case, the younger the better)

So if it’s not Pick a larva, any larva’, what do the bees choose to start their emergency queen cells from?

Remember how important this is. Without a new queen the colony cannot survive. The clock is ticking. They only have a few days to make this choice before all the brood in the nest are too old for queen production.

The non-random construction of queen cells.

They predominantly choose eggs.

Almost 70% of queen cells started were initiated when the cell contained an egg, rather than a larva. What’s more, the majority of the eggs chosen were three days old.

If you consider that there were 6 possible choices (1, 2 or 3 day old eggs and 1, 2 and 3 day old larvae), it’s striking that 34% of all the queen cells produced were from 3 day old eggs.

In fact, it turns out that only five choices were made as none of the queen cells were started from 3 day old larvae.

Furthermore, over 60% of queen cells produced from 2 day old larvae were subsequently torn down.

Bees choose to make queens from the oldest eggs or the very youngest larvae.

Are you getting the message?

Since the production of a new queen is essential for colony survival we should assume that the bees have evolved a queen cell production ‘strategy’ that maximises the chances of producing a suitable queen.

Almost 60% of the ‘starting material’ chosen by the bees to ensure colony survival – that resulted in queen production – were 3 day old eggs or 1 day old larvae.

This emphasises the need to provide colonies we use for queen rearing with eggs and larvae of this age range. It also reinforces the importance of only selecting the smallest larvae possible when grafting.

The choice the bees make is presumably because queens reared from older larvae are of poorer quality, perhaps because they have a reduced period for feeding with Royal Jelly.

So how do the queens produced from eggs and young larvae compare?

Queen ‘quality’

Of the 91 queens that emerged only 89 were analysed because two ”escaped capture”.

It’s reassuring to know that it’s not just cackhanded beekeepers that make mistakes 😉

There were no differences in the morphology – weight or size – for queens that emerged from cells on either the central or peripheral frames 10.

However, queens reared from 3 day old eggs were significantly heavier than queens reared from larvae. In addition, queens reared from 3 day old eggs had a longer thorax than queens reared from either younger eggs or larvae.

Other morphological measurement – e.g. wing length or width – did not differ significantly between queens reared from eggs or larvae.

But are these hefty, long-thoraxed, queens better quality?

This isn’t a simple question. What does better quality mean? It’s not the size or productivity of the resulting colony she heads since that is also influenced by the genetics and number of drones she mates with.

It’s also time consuming and impractical to measure scientifically (for 89 queens).

Instead, the scientists measured the number of ovarioles and the volume of the spermatheca as potential indicators of fecundity. There was no relationship between weight and ovariole number, irrespective of the age of the egg or larva when the cell was started.

If not more fecund, what?

So, if bigger queens don’t necessarily have increased fecundity (though remember, this wasn’t shown – all they demonstrated was that the ‘innards’ involved in fertilised egg production were similar) why might the bees select eggs/larvae that resulted in bigger queens being produced?

One possibility is that these bigger queens have greater success in what is termed polygyny reduction.

This is what beekeepers call fighting.

If more than one queen is present they fight until only one is left in the hive. This hadn’t been extensively studied in 1999 (when this paper was published) but has been addressed in other studies 11.

Alternatively, and suggested in a tempting but cryptic ’unpublished data’, heavier queens may be able to achieve higher levels of polyandry i.e. mate with more drones, so increasing the genetic diversity, and consequently the fitness, of the colony. I’ve discussed the importance of polyandry and so-called hyperpolyandry for colony fitness and disease resistance previously, so won’t revisit these here.

It’s easy to speculate that a queen with a larger thorax may have better developed flight muscles. These might enable her to stay longer in drone congregation areas for mating.

Why are so many cells started (and queens reared)?

In the emergency response only one queen is needed to ‘rescue’ the colony from oblivion.

Why therefore are so many queen cells – on average 27 per colony – started?

And why do the workers allow an average of 11 queens emerge?

The authors suggest a number of possible reasons:

  1. Colonies raise multiple queens to guarantee the requeening process. This assumes that the ‘cost’ of queen rearing is low, which seems reasonable. Since only 5% of queens raised failed to emerge it is probably not to overcome this limitation.
  2. Multiple queens allow colony reproduction if conditions are suitable. Only colonies that raise multiple queens would be able to (simultaneously) reproduce and requeen, so there might be a selective pressure to allow this.
  3. A consequence of age demographics (brood or workers) in the colony. This is slightly trickier to explain and has not been tested. Queen cells result from an ‘interaction’ of available brood (eggs/larvae) with workers. A colony has variable numbers of both, and there are a variety of worker cohorts, only some of which contribute to cell building. Therefore, the production of multiple cells (and queens) may simply reflect the variation in the factors – ages of brood and workers – involved.
  4. Rearing multiple queens allows workers to select the ‘best’. That’s clearly wrong because the ‘best’ would be just one queen. Perhaps a better explanation would be that it allows workers to either select for better queens by destroying those that are less good.

No single reason

Biology is complicated 12 and it may be that all four of the reasons above are correct. There may be (and almost certainly are) additional reasons that favour the production of multiple queens.

However, of the four reasons above, this paper provides nearly compelling evidence that the workers are selecting which emerge and which do not.

Remember, 53% of the cells that were started were torn down.

In addition, there was both a spatial and temporal bias to the cells that were torn down. This strongly suggests that the process (of cell destruction) was not random.

However, it remains only nearly compelling because we know nothing about the queens that were in cells that were torn down.

By definition those queens don’t exist. The cells were torn down and the queens killed/eaten/discarded so we have no measure of their quality.

If they were indistinguishable from those that did emerge then I’d struggle to convince you that the worker selection was producing ‘better queens’ from the large number of queen cells that were started.

Analysing the non-existent

But fortunately this experiment has been done.

Tarpy, D.R., Simone-Finstrom, M. & Linksvayer, T.A. Honey bee colonies regulate queen reproductive traits by controlling which queens survive to adulthood. Insect. Soc. 63, 169–174 (2016).

The experimental methods were almost identical. However, this time, when they caged the capped queen cells they randomly assigned them to cages that either allowed or prevented worker access (both types of cages prevented the escape of the queen).

They then analysed the queens that emerged from the ‘worker-accessible’ and ‘worker-excluded’ queen cells.

The hypothesis was straightforward, if the workers were randomly destroying a proportion of queen cells there would be no differences in the characteristics of the resulting queens. Conversely, if there was selection, the queens from the ‘worker-excluded’ cells would be different.

The overall numbers of queen cells produced (average 12, range 4 – 22 per colony) and the proportion – 57% – of the ‘worker-accessible’ cells torn down were similar to the study I’ve already described.

Effect of queen treatment on two different measures of queen reproductive potential.

‘Worker-excluded’ queens were significantly smaller than those from ‘worker-accessible’ cages. They also weighed less. This is obvious from the top left panel (above) but confounded 13 by the small size of the study and the significant differences in the weight of queens produced in different colonies 14.

Despite the limited size of this study these results strongly suggest that workers are somehow ‘weeding out’ lower quality (defined here as smaller and probably lighter) queens.

I’ll leave it to you to speculate on how the workers outside the queen cell determine the size/weight/quality of the queen inside the cell … 😉

Does this have relevance to beekeeping?

I think there are a number of interesting points from this study that have relevance to practical beekeeping.

  • Queen cells were started under the emergency response only in the first 3 days after the queen was removed. The vast majority were started within 24 hours. This should help determine when the queen went missing or – if you deliberately removed her – defines the latest date that you need to be concerned about new cells being started.
  • If you are improving your stocks by adding larvae from a separate colony 15 then make sure you add a frame containing eggs and larvae. You want to be sure they have access to 3 day old eggs.
  • It probably makes sense to place this frame in the centre of the brood nest.
  • If you’re grafting larvae for queen rearing – as I’ve already suggested – make sure you choose those under ~18 hours old. The younger and the smaller the better.
  • But, perhaps we should instead think about grafting eggs rather than larvae?

This last suggestion is a topic of a (part-written) future post.

Here are a couple of additional points to think about. Studies have shown that egg transfer results in the largest queens. However, eggs are accepted significantly less well than larvae … and some colonies will not accept them at all. I’ll discuss this in more detail some other time.

And a final caveat …

The final point to remember is that both these studies analysed queen cell production and the resulting queens under the emergency response.

Many queen rearing methods – the so-called ‘queenright’ ones such as my favoured Ben Harden method – exploit the supersedure response. It’s always possible that the bees have different preferences for queens reared under the supersedure (or for that matter the swarming) responses.

But I doubt it 😉

After all … colony survival is dependent upon good quality queens and the bees know best.


 

Footnotes

  1. See this post as well for more details.
  2. Usual caveats apply … I’m writing this in the Northern hemisphere as we segue from spring to summer.
  3. But don’t rely on this!
  4. Though not exclusively … ahem!
  5. Or several as you will see below.
  6. As an aside, I can’t think of a frequent and natural cause of sudden queen disappearance from a hive. Disease perhaps, but injury or sudden death seems unlikely. What have I forgotten?
  7. Removal of a leg, for example.
  8. I’ll briefly return to any relevance to the supersedure and swarming responses, and to queen rearing at the end.
  9. A conservative estimate for May and June.
  10. After age matching those started from eggs or larvae.
  11. The enthusiastic could read this for starters, but I’ll probably deal with it sometime in the future.
  12. Which quite neatly summarises my career.
  13. In terms of statistical significance.
  14. Only 33 queens were reared overall, 15 in ‘worker-excluded’ and 18 in ‘worker-accessible’ cages.
  15. Having destroyed all the queen cells produced from the previous queen’s brood.

20 thoughts on “The bees know best

  1. Elaine Robinson

    David, this is a fantastic, insightful piece this week. Thanks for sharing. I’ve been thinking (a lot) about queen rearing as well as trying different techniques and recently considering whether some of the more ‘best practise’ techniques, are actually the best e.g. grafting. I’ve been trying some simple methods this year, rather than grafting, such as putting an older comb (easy to cut delicate cells from) for the queen to lay on, then adding to a queenless colony and letting the bees choose the best genetic material. I’d read previously, about rare patrilines they choose to ensure colony fitness, made sense to let them choose rather than me. I wonder if this is another ‘implication’ / tip from your post? Rather than the beekeeper choosing the v young larva to rear a queen via grafting, let the bees choose, then cut out the cell from the comb and add to your mating Nuc.
    Thanks for such an interesting post this week.

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hi Elaine

      I wrote about the rare royal patrilines a couple of years ago. There are other studies that suggest alternative factors – other than genetic rarity per se – may actually be more important and I’ll write about this sometime later this year.

      Like you I’ve been tinkering with the way I rear queens. I’ve attempted using cell punching and am investigating egg transfer. I find grafting my ‘local Fife mongrels’ very easy and routinely get 80-90% take (or better on a good day). However, my dark native bees on the west coast are a different proposition, where 50-60% is more normal. Gilles Fert (in Raising Honeybee Queens) also comments on difficulty working with Apis mellifera mellifera. I’m therefore looking at ways to increase ‘take’. It might be the cell raiser rather than the way I choose and introduce larvae … who knows?

      Have fun,
      David

      Reply
      1. John Bolger

        Fantastic stuff. Thank you.

        One thing strikes me about the percentage “take”. For a small scale beekeeper who just wants 3-4 queens I’m not sure why a high percentage is important. If we supply 20 grafts (or whatever) and there’s 50% take and then the bees cull to five then isn’t that just hunky dory.

        Interestingly, I have always wondered why the general recommendation is to graft a very recently hatched larva rather than an egg. After all an egg will hatch pretty soon if that’s what the bees prefer. Similarly, with this new evidence that bees will raise queen cells from a three day old egg, it will be difficult to identify an egg of that age.
        But of course any egg that is not three days old when grafted will reach that age pretty soon.
        (Why do I feel I’m missing something here)

        Thanks again.

        John

        Reply
        1. David Post author

          Hi John

          I’ll discuss eggs later in the season. I don’t want to give the game away here. There are some relatively easy ways to get eggs of the right age, should you want them.

          You’re right about small-scale beekeepers not needing 20 queens at a time, and if only 4-5 ‘take’ then that’s OK. However, grafting 20 takes four times as long as doing 5, so more chances of them drying out and a bit more disruption for the colony. I’d prefer to have a really good percentage ‘take’ and only have to graft a small number to be sure I was going to get what I need. Usually I do 10 at a time. It suits my cell raisers and it’s not too many for me to deal with for mating. I also prefer to do repeat batches as the weather cannot be relied upon for mating, so good not to put all your ‘eggs in one basket’.

          Cheers
          David

          Reply
          1. Adam Darling

            Once again, thanks for an insightful article.
            FWIW, I too graft 10 at a time for the same reasons as yourself. In a queenright colony, using the supercedure response, the first attempt usually results in a poor uptake. Later ones are better.
            It’s not uncommon for a colony to be left with what the beekeeper thinks is just one queencell, only to find that the bees have made a very very late one – sometimes towards the side of the brood nest on an outer frame. I wonder if a cooler part of the hive results in slower development of the egg? It’s worthwhile going back a week or so after the desired queencell is sealed and just before emergence, to check on this.
            I have also witnessed on several occasions that workers will move eggs to a queencell above the excluder, so there must be some choice as to which egg they chose? And this would demonstrate that it’s not necessarily the queen that places an egg in a queencell.

          2. David Post author

            Hi Adam

            My first attempts at grafting each season are usually poor, with improved ‘take’ on later repeats. Whether this is due to the colonies not being in the right state, or me being a bit rusty isn’t clear.

            I only return once to knock back queen cells, one week after the previous visit. Perhaps this is what you were meaning – I’ve certainly discussed this before, for example in the post three weeks ago. Assuming I left a charged cell at (a ‘reactive’ split) the previous visit there will be no more larvae of a suitable age one week later. If they choose one after that it’s unlikely to properly develop and the selected queen’s appearance should terminate any further activity. I’m certainly not saying it doesn’t happen, but it’s not something I’ve had a problem with – assuming there’s a full 7 days between visits. With a ‘proactive’ split i.e. no queen cells present when I remove the queen, it’s reassuring to know I’ve selected a charged cell and I can do this by returning before 7 days are up, knowing the the majority of cells are started within the first 24 hours. I select one, and rub out the rest. The later you leave this the less chance they’ll start any more, though – at least mathematically – they can until there are no larva less than 3 days old.

            I’ve been meaning to write about ‘workers moving eggs’ for ages. The reason I haven’t is that I’m not convinced they do. Here’s one I found on Monday:

            Queen cell in the super

            Did the bees move it there? Or did a laying worker – of which there are always a small number in the hive – lay the egg? The pupa wasn’t fully developed but looked to me like a drone. This makes me suspect it was laid by a worker, rather than being moved (which would indicate there is no selection). I need to read more about the topic before I write more … but here’s another (photo from 2014):

            Billy no mates

            I included this in a 2018 post that briefly discussed brood in the supers. As I said in a footnote to that post “I’m loathe to say always drones as someone will inevitably prove me wrong … however, I don’t remember ever finding individual worker brood above the queen excluder in the supers.”

            Cheers
            David

  2. Jo horsley

    Hi David, another fantastic, interesting blog, thank you. I videoed a worker rapidly tapping a queen cell with her tail yesterday, we have all seen them fussing cells in this way. I assumed it was some sort of communication but your blog made me wonder could she be judging how full the cell is, and therefore how big the queen, by the sound, much as we would judge how full a settling tank is by tapping the side?

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hi Jo

      I don’t know how the queens are ‘judged’. Perhaps they do tap on the side of the cell. The other thing to remember is the noises ‘soon to emerge’ queens make. Perhaps the frequency is an indication of quality/size? They don’t have details on when the queen cells were torn down.

      Cheers
      David

      Reply
  3. Simon

    I’ve had a number of colonies suddenly go queenless this year, with no visible or visual sign of the presence of the queen, I.e. no eggs. Interestingly adding a frame of young larvae and eggs in a couple of instances produced no emergency queen cells. In these instances I’ve suspected supersedure, but so far no signs of a newly mated queen starting to lay eggs, nor any obvious queen cells. Any thoughts what might be going on David please?

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hi Simon

      If it’s supersedure you should expect the old queen should go on laying – perhaps at a reduced rate – while the new queen mates and starts laying. There’s usually no break in brood production (at least in my experience).

      The absence of QC’s on the introduced frame is usually pretty conclusive evidence that the colony is queenright, though she may be a virgin or temporarily not laying.

      I bet there are queens in the boxes … has a really strong nectar flow stopped and are you now in the ‘June gap’? It’s not unusual for the colony to stop rearing new brood if there’s no nectar coming in. Try giving them 250 ml of thin syrup via a contact feeder every other day in the hope that it tricks them into thinking there’s some nectar coming in?

      Cheers
      David

      Reply
    2. Dani

      I’ve had two colonies this year produce EQCs only after two test frames. I’ve always considered that a positive response is indicative of the absence of a queen while a negative can prove nothing.

      Reply
      1. David Post author

        Hello Dani

        Interesting. I see your point about a negative proving nothing but don’t understand why the bees would want to wait. Presumably the two frames were added 6+ days apart to allow all brood on the first to progress from the youngest eggs to 3+ day old larvae. The colony ‘knows’ it’s queenless within a very short period and – as shown in one of the papers above – starts the majority of queen cells within 24 hours of removal of the queen. I can’t think of a sensible reason that would make them delay …

        I can think of some not sensible ones … there was a virgin in the box who failed to return from a mating flight, so the bees ‘thought’ they were queenright, but subsequently were not. Alternatively, if there was open brood on the frames added, perhaps the brood pheromone suppressed a low level of laying worker activity that gave the colony the feeling that is was queenright.

        Both highly unlikely 🙁

        Maybe they were just being contrary 🙂

        Cheers
        David

        Reply
  4. Maccon Keane

    Dear David.

    Thank you for a really interesting post. My question is this. Using the nucleus method of swarm control by queen removal and induction of the emergency response the beekeeper has to select a queen cell to head the original colony. From these data there is a one in 20 chance (5%) that the chosen cell will not emerge. This is a problem but low risk. However there is a one in two (50%) chance that the beekeeper will select a cell that the bees would have torn down and therefore actively select a lesser quality queen. For an individual colony this may not be particularly significant but over a few generations this negative selective pressure (50%) against the best quality Queens will rapidly lead to a deterioration in stock compared to that which would have happened had the queen been chosen by the bees themselves. Can you think of any way to avoid introducing this systematic negative selection pressure to ensure we let the bees choose the queen because as you title the piece ‘the bees know best’?

    Yours sincerely

    Maccon Keane

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hi Maccon

      I was wondering if any readers would pick this up 😉

      Rather than responding here I’m going to take the liberty of providing a more expansive answer in my post this Friday which also deals with some other aspects of queen rearing.

      I hope that’s OK? And many thanks for the coffee 🙂

      Cheers
      David

      Reply
  5. Maccon Keane

    David
    I am just really chuffed that the answer to my question requires a full post. I feel I’ve just gone to the top of the class. Really looking forward to Friday’s post. I expect you can write it while enjoying your coffee.
    Keep well
    Maccon

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      I can’t promise a full post (unless I write 5000 words, as there’s some other things I want to cover) … just a full answer 😉
      Cheers
      David

      Reply
  6. David hancock

    Hi David, thank you too for yet another interesting post. I don’t know how you keep coming up with them!
    After your article last Friday I went to check on one of my hives that I had separated using your nuc method ( my standard) after 3 weeks only to find no queen, no brood but a big lovely queen cell unopened….. when I looked inside it did look like a drone ( but what do I know). First time it has happened to me. My issue is how best to recombine with the nuc. When choosing the new queen in the original hive all is easy. Dispose of old queen before recombination. The other way round is a right faff. Add separators to move bees off the supers, then a day or so later, use newspaper method , then go back in and sort it out. Is there a good way to do this? I end up putting a queen excluder on a base, adding a brood box, lifting all the ex-nuc frames into that box, then start removing frames from the hive brood box , all the time looking for the queen and not always finding her, constantly worrying that I might drop her outside of the hive in transit, or damage her in the process… have you covered this before or is it an idea for a post for us relative newbies ( 3 hrs in, 3 hives, carniolans, 1hive of which absconded last week of April before I even looked ☹️)

    Reply
    1. David Post author

      Hi David

      If I understand the Q you’re wondering how to combine a queenright nuc with a queenless colony (from which it was split). All the bees are therefore from the same queen. That being the case they should not fight and so you shouldn’t need newspaper.

      There are a variety of ways to achieve this, the main problem being the difficulty of balancing supers on top of a nuc box. I’d be tempted to move the frames from the nuc box into the middle of a full brood box, then fill the remaining space with either fat dummies or empty frames. I’d place this box below the full brood box on the original stand, with the supers on top as usual. That assumes the nuc was moved back from an out apiary.

      If the nuc is in the same apiary I’d place the ‘nuc frames in a full brood box’ above the original brood box. The fact that the workers have to negotiate the full box to get to the entrance should be sufficient to tell them that their location has changed.

      If I’ve misinterpreted, or you want to unite with a nuc containing a queen other than the original, you can always use newspaper above and below the ‘new’ queenright box. In this case the setup would be floor-queenless brood box-newspaper-queenright nuc in a full brood box-newspaper-queen excluder-supers.

      When you remove the frames from the nuc box the queen will almost certainly be on the face of one of the frames. Do things gently and she’ll stay there. Look at each frame and you’ll probably see her. If you don’t, check the remaining bees in the – now empty – nuc box. If she’s not there you’ll almost certainly have missed her on one of the frames. Knock the bees into the brood box (better) or in front of the hive. I’ve done this dozens of times and never had a problem.

      When you transfer the frames from the nuc don’t worry about the queen falling onto the ground. Queens have a remarkable self-preservation drive and they do their very best to stay on the face of brood containing frames. If necessary, place the brood box on a nearby roof. She’ll only leave the frame if you drop it on the ground or shake it vigorously.

      Good luck (though you won’t need it!)
      David

      Reply

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *